Skip to content

Research at St Andrews

The early emergence of guilt-motivated prosocial behavior

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

DOI

Open Access Status

  • Embargoed (until 7/11/18)

Author(s)

Amrisha Vaish, Malinda Carpenter, Michael Tomasello

School/Research organisations

Abstract

Guilt serves vital prosocial functions: It motivates transgressors to make amends, thus restoring damaged relationships. Previous developmental research on guilt has not clearly distinguished it from sympathy for a victim or a tendency to repair damage in general. The authors tested 2- and 3-year-old children (N = 62 and 64, respectively) in a 2 × 2 design, varying whether or not a mishap caused harm to someone and whether children themselves caused that mishap. Three-year-olds showed greatest reparative behavior when they had caused the mishap and it caused harm, thus showing a specific effect of guilt. Two-year-olds repaired more whenever harm was caused, no matter by whom, thus showing only an effect of sympathy. Guilt as a distinct motivator of prosocial behavior thus emerges by at least 3 years.
Close

Details

Original languageEnglish
Pages (from-to)1772-1782
Number of pages11
JournalChild Development
Volume87
Issue number6
DOIs
StatePublished - 7 Nov 2016

    Research areas

  • Guilt, Prosocial behavior, Sympathy

Discover related content
Find related publications, people, projects and more using interactive charts.

View graph of relations

Related by author

  1. Communicative eye contact signals a commitment to cooperate for young children

    Siposova, B., Tomasello, M. & Carpenter, M. 28 Jun 2018 In : Cognition. 179, p. 192-201

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  2. Joint action and joint attention: drawing parallels between the literatures

    Milward, S. & Carpenter, M. 26 Mar 2018 In : Social and Personality Psychology Compass. Early View, e12377

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  3. The whistleblower’s dilemma in young children: when loyalty trumps other moral concerns

    Misch, A., Over, H. & Carpenter, M. 1 Mar 2018 In : Frontiers in Psychology. 9, 9 p., 250

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  4. Selective copying of the majority suggests children are broadly "optimal-" rather than "over-" imitators

    Evans, C. L., Laland, K. N., Carpenter, M. & Kendal, R. L. 17 Dec 2017 In : Developmental Science. Early View, e12637

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  5. Great apes distinguish true from false beliefs in an interactive helping task

    Buttelmann, D., Buttelmann, F., Carpenter, M., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. 5 Apr 2017 In : PLoS One. 12, 4, 13 p., e0173793

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

Related by journal

  1. The development of selective copying: children's learning from an expert versus their mother

    Lucas, A. J., Burdett, E. R. R., Burgess, V., Wood, L. A., McGuigan, N., Harris, P. L. & Whiten, A. 7 Nov 2017 In : Child Development. 88, 6, p. 2026-2042 17 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  2. Imitation, collaboration, and their interaction among Western and Indigenous Australian preschool children

    Nielsen, M., Mushin, I., Tomaselli, K. & Whiten, A. 18 May 2016 In : Child Development. 87, 3, p. 795-806 12 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  3. Learning from their own actions: the unique effect of producing actions on infants’ action understanding

    Gerson, S. & Woodward, A. Feb 2014 In : Child Development. 85, 1, p. 264-277

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  4. Where Culture Takes Hold: "Overimitation" and Its Flexible Deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen Children

    Nielsen, M., Mushin, I., Tomaselli, K. & Whiten, A. 1 Nov 2014 In : Child Development. 85, 6, p. 2169-2184 16 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

ID: 214244841