Skip to content

Research at St Andrews

The evolution and cultural transmission of percussive technology: integrating evidence from paleoanthropology and primatology

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

DOI

Author(s)

Andrew Whiten, Kathy Schick, Nicholas Toth

School/Research organisations

Abstract

We bring together the quite different kinds of evidence available from palaeoanthropology and primatology to better understand the origins of Plio-Pleistocene percussive technology. Accumulated palaeoanthropological discoveries now document the Oldowan Complex as the dominant stone tool making culture between 2.6-1.4 Ma, the earlier part of this contemporaneous with pre-Homo hominins. The principal types of artefacts and other remains from 20 Early Stone Age (Oldowan and earliest Acheulean) localities in Africa and elsewhere are reviewed and described. To better understand the ancestral behavioural foundations of this early lithic culture, we examine a range of recent findings from primatology. In particular, we attempt to identify key shared characteristics of Homo and Pan that support inferences about the preparedness of our common ancestor for the innovation of stone tool culture. Findings of particular relevance include: (i) the discovery of an expanding repertoire of percussive and other tool use based on directed use of force among wild chimpanzees, implicating capacities that include significant innovatory potential and appreciation of relevant causal factors; (ii) evidence of material cultural diversity among wild chimpanzees, indicating a readiness to acquire and transmit tool use innovations; and (iii) experimental studies of social learning in chimpanzees and bonobos that now encompass the acquisition of nut cracking through observation of skilled use of hammers and anvils by conspecifics, the diffusion within and between groups of alternative styles of tool use, and the adoption of free-hand stone-to-stone percussion to create useful flakes for cutting to gain access to food resources. We use the distributions of the inferred cultural traits in the wild to assess how diffusion relates to geographic distances, and find that shared traits drop by 50% from the approximately eight characteristic of close neighbours over a distance of approximately 700 km. This pattern is used to explore the implications of analogous processes operating in relation to Early Stone Age sites. (C) 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Close

Details

Original languageEnglish
Pages (from-to)420-435
Number of pages16
JournalJournal of Human Evolution
Volume57
Issue number4
DOIs
StatePublished - Oct 2009

    Research areas

  • Stone tools, Tool making, Social learning, Oldowan, Early Stone Age, Plio-Pleistocene, Pan, Chimpanzee, Bonobo, CHIMPANZEES PAN-TROGLODYTES, MONKEYS CEBUS-APELLA, PLIOCENE ARCHAEOLOGICAL SITES, CHILDREN HOMO-SAPIENS, STONE TOOL-MAKING, WILD CHIMPANZEES, LOWER PLEISTOCENE, EARLY HOMINID, WEST TURKANA, OLDUVAI-GORGE

Discover related content
Find related publications, people, projects and more using interactive charts.

View graph of relations

Related by author

  1. Innovation and social transmission in experimental micro-societies: exploring the scope of cumulative culture in young children

    McGuigan, N., Burdett, E., Burgess, V., Dean, L., Lucas, A., Vale, G. & Whiten, A. 23 Oct 2017 In : Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society. B, Biological Sciences. 372, 1735, 14 p., 20160425

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  2. A second inheritance system: the extension of biology through culture

    Whiten, A. 6 Oct 2017 In : Interface Focus. 7, 5, 16 p., 20160142

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  3. Acquisition of a socially learned tool use sequence in chimpanzees: implications for cumulative culture

    Vale, G. L., Davis, S. J., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J. & Whiten, A. Sep 2017 In : Evolution and Human Behavior. 38, 5, p. 635-644 10 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  4. Resilience of experimentally-seeded dietary traditions in wild vervets: evidence from group fissions

    van de Waal, E., van Schaik, C. P. & Whiten, A. 1 Aug 2017 In : American Journal of Primatology. Early View

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  5. Adaptive cultural transmission biases in children and nonhuman primates

    Price, E. E., Wood, L. A. & Whiten, A. Aug 2017 In : Infant Behavior and Development. 48, Part A, p. 45-53 9 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalReview article

Related by journal

  1. Selection to outsmart the germs: the evolution of disease recognition and social cognition

    Kessler, S. E., Bonnell, T. R., Byrne, R. W. & Chapman, C. A. 17 May 2017 In : Journal of Human Evolution. 108, p. 92-109 18 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  2. Climatic variability, plasticity, and dispersal: a case study from Lake Tana, Ethiopia

    Grove, M., Lamb, H., Roberts, H., Davies, S., Marshall, M., Bates, C. R. & Huws, D. Oct 2015 In : Journal of Human Evolution. 87, p. 32-47 16 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  3. How old are chimpanzee communities? Time to the most recent common ancestor of the Y-chromosome in highly patrilocal societies

    Langergraber, K., Rowney, C., Schubert, G., Crockford, C., Hobaiter, C., Wittig, R., Wrangham, R. W., Zuberbuhler, K. & Vigilant, L. Apr 2014 In : Journal of Human Evolution. 69, p. 1-7

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  4. Population trajectories for accidental versus planned colonisation of islands

    Ruxton, G. D. & Wilkinson, D. M. Sep 2012 In : Journal of Human Evolution. 63, 3, p. 507-511 5 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  5. Thermoregulation and endurance running in extinct hominins: Wheeler's models revisited

    Ruxton, G. D. & Wilkinson, D. M. Aug 2011 In : Journal of Human Evolution. 61, 2, p. 169-175 7 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

ID: 449763