Skip to content

Research at St Andrews

Where Culture Takes Hold: "Overimitation" and Its Flexible Deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen Children

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

DOI

Standard

Where Culture Takes Hold : "Overimitation" and Its Flexible Deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen Children. / Nielsen, M.; Mushin, I.; Tomaselli, K.; Whiten, A.

In: Child Development, Vol. 85, No. 6, 01.11.2014, p. 2169-2184.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

Harvard

Nielsen, M, Mushin, I, Tomaselli, K & Whiten, A 2014, 'Where Culture Takes Hold: "Overimitation" and Its Flexible Deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen Children' Child Development, vol 85, no. 6, pp. 2169-2184. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12265

APA

Nielsen, M., Mushin, I., Tomaselli, K., & Whiten, A. (2014). Where Culture Takes Hold: "Overimitation" and Its Flexible Deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen Children. Child Development, 85(6), 2169-2184. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12265

Vancouver

Nielsen M, Mushin I, Tomaselli K, Whiten A. Where Culture Takes Hold: "Overimitation" and Its Flexible Deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen Children. Child Development. 2014 Nov 1;85(6):2169-2184. Available from, DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12265

Author

Nielsen, M.; Mushin, I.; Tomaselli, K.; Whiten, A. / Where Culture Takes Hold : "Overimitation" and Its Flexible Deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen Children.

In: Child Development, Vol. 85, No. 6, 01.11.2014, p. 2169-2184.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

Bibtex - Download

@article{30c0f6448929445397535173d01d5342,
title = "Where Culture Takes Hold: {"}Overimitation{"} and Its Flexible Deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen Children",
abstract = "Children often {"}overimitate,{"} comprehensively copying others' actions despite manifest perceptual cues to their causal ineffectuality. The inflexibility of this behavior renders its adaptive significance difficult to apprehend. This study explored the boundaries of overimitation in 3- to 6-year-old children of three distinct cultures: Westernized, urban Australians (N = 64 in Experiment 1; N = 19 in Experiment 2) and remote communities of South African Bushmen (N = 64) and Australian Aborigines (N = 19). Children overimitated at high frequency in all communities and generalized what they had learned about techniques and object affordances from one object to another. Overimitation thus provides a powerful means of acquiring and flexibly deploying cultural knowledge. The potency of such social learning was also documented compared to opportunities for exploration and practice.",
author = "M. Nielsen and I. Mushin and K. Tomaselli and A. Whiten",
year = "2014",
month = "11",
doi = "10.1111/cdev.12265",
volume = "85",
pages = "2169--2184",
journal = "Child Development",
issn = "0009-3920",
publisher = "Wiley-Blackwell",
number = "6",

}

RIS (suitable for import to EndNote) - Download

TY - JOUR

T1 - Where Culture Takes Hold

T2 - Child Development

AU - Nielsen,M.

AU - Mushin,I.

AU - Tomaselli,K.

AU - Whiten,A.

PY - 2014/11/1

Y1 - 2014/11/1

N2 - Children often "overimitate," comprehensively copying others' actions despite manifest perceptual cues to their causal ineffectuality. The inflexibility of this behavior renders its adaptive significance difficult to apprehend. This study explored the boundaries of overimitation in 3- to 6-year-old children of three distinct cultures: Westernized, urban Australians (N = 64 in Experiment 1; N = 19 in Experiment 2) and remote communities of South African Bushmen (N = 64) and Australian Aborigines (N = 19). Children overimitated at high frequency in all communities and generalized what they had learned about techniques and object affordances from one object to another. Overimitation thus provides a powerful means of acquiring and flexibly deploying cultural knowledge. The potency of such social learning was also documented compared to opportunities for exploration and practice.

AB - Children often "overimitate," comprehensively copying others' actions despite manifest perceptual cues to their causal ineffectuality. The inflexibility of this behavior renders its adaptive significance difficult to apprehend. This study explored the boundaries of overimitation in 3- to 6-year-old children of three distinct cultures: Westernized, urban Australians (N = 64 in Experiment 1; N = 19 in Experiment 2) and remote communities of South African Bushmen (N = 64) and Australian Aborigines (N = 19). Children overimitated at high frequency in all communities and generalized what they had learned about techniques and object affordances from one object to another. Overimitation thus provides a powerful means of acquiring and flexibly deploying cultural knowledge. The potency of such social learning was also documented compared to opportunities for exploration and practice.

U2 - 10.1111/cdev.12265

DO - 10.1111/cdev.12265

M3 - Article

VL - 85

SP - 2169

EP - 2184

JO - Child Development

JF - Child Development

SN - 0009-3920

IS - 6

ER -

Related by author

  1. Innovation and social transmission in experimental micro-societies: exploring the scope of cumulative culture in young children

    McGuigan, N., Burdett, E., Burgess, V., Dean, L., Lucas, A., Vale, G. & Whiten, A. 5 Dec 2017 In : Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society. B, Biological Sciences. 372, 1735, 14 p., 20160425

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  2. A second inheritance system: the extension of biology through culture

    Whiten, A. 6 Oct 2017 In : Interface Focus. 7, 5, 16 p., 20160142

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  3. Acquisition of a socially learned tool use sequence in chimpanzees: implications for cumulative culture

    Vale, G. L., Davis, S. J., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J. & Whiten, A. Sep 2017 In : Evolution and Human Behavior. 38, 5, p. 635-644 10 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  4. Resilience of experimentally-seeded dietary traditions in wild vervets: evidence from group fissions

    van de Waal, E., van Schaik, C. P. & Whiten, A. 1 Aug 2017 In : American Journal of Primatology. Early View

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  5. Adaptive cultural transmission biases in children and nonhuman primates

    Price, E. E., Wood, L. A. & Whiten, A. Aug 2017 In : Infant Behavior and Development. 48, Part A, p. 45-53 9 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalReview article

Related by journal

  1. The development of selective copying: children's learning from an expert versus their mother

    Lucas, A. J., Burdett, E. R. R., Burgess, V., Wood, L. A., McGuigan, N., Harris, P. L. & Whiten, A. 29 Dec 2016 In : Child Development. Early View, 17 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  2. The early emergence of guilt-motivated prosocial behavior

    Vaish, A., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. 7 Nov 2016 In : Child Development. 87, 6, p. 1772-1782 11 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  3. Imitation, collaboration, and their interaction among Western and Indigenous Australian preschool children

    Nielsen, M., Mushin, I., Tomaselli, K. & Whiten, A. 18 May 2016 In : Child Development. 87, 3, p. 795-806 12 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  4. Learning from their own actions: the unique effect of producing actions on infants’ action understanding

    Gerson, S. & Woodward, A. Feb 2014 In : Child Development. 85, 1, p. 264-277

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

ID: 159773130