Skip to content

Research at St Andrews

Where Culture Takes Hold: "Overimitation" and Its Flexible Deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen Children

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

DOI

Standard

Where Culture Takes Hold : "Overimitation" and Its Flexible Deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen Children. / Nielsen, M.; Mushin, I.; Tomaselli, K.; Whiten, A.

In: Child Development, Vol. 85, No. 6, 01.11.2014, p. 2169-2184.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

Harvard

Nielsen, M, Mushin, I, Tomaselli, K & Whiten, A 2014, 'Where Culture Takes Hold: "Overimitation" and Its Flexible Deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen Children' Child Development, vol. 85, no. 6, pp. 2169-2184. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12265

APA

Nielsen, M., Mushin, I., Tomaselli, K., & Whiten, A. (2014). Where Culture Takes Hold: "Overimitation" and Its Flexible Deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen Children. Child Development, 85(6), 2169-2184. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12265

Vancouver

Nielsen M, Mushin I, Tomaselli K, Whiten A. Where Culture Takes Hold: "Overimitation" and Its Flexible Deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen Children. Child Development. 2014 Nov 1;85(6):2169-2184. Available from, DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12265

Author

Nielsen, M. ; Mushin, I. ; Tomaselli, K. ; Whiten, A./ Where Culture Takes Hold : "Overimitation" and Its Flexible Deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen Children. In: Child Development. 2014 ; Vol. 85, No. 6. pp. 2169-2184

Bibtex - Download

@article{30c0f6448929445397535173d01d5342,
title = "Where Culture Takes Hold: {"}Overimitation{"} and Its Flexible Deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen Children",
abstract = "Children often {"}overimitate,{"} comprehensively copying others' actions despite manifest perceptual cues to their causal ineffectuality. The inflexibility of this behavior renders its adaptive significance difficult to apprehend. This study explored the boundaries of overimitation in 3- to 6-year-old children of three distinct cultures: Westernized, urban Australians (N = 64 in Experiment 1; N = 19 in Experiment 2) and remote communities of South African Bushmen (N = 64) and Australian Aborigines (N = 19). Children overimitated at high frequency in all communities and generalized what they had learned about techniques and object affordances from one object to another. Overimitation thus provides a powerful means of acquiring and flexibly deploying cultural knowledge. The potency of such social learning was also documented compared to opportunities for exploration and practice.",
author = "M. Nielsen and I. Mushin and K. Tomaselli and A. Whiten",
year = "2014",
month = "11",
day = "1",
doi = "10.1111/cdev.12265",
language = "English",
volume = "85",
pages = "2169--2184",
journal = "Child Development",
issn = "0009-3920",
publisher = "Wiley-Blackwell",
number = "6",

}

RIS (suitable for import to EndNote) - Download

TY - JOUR

T1 - Where Culture Takes Hold

T2 - Child Development

AU - Nielsen,M.

AU - Mushin,I.

AU - Tomaselli,K.

AU - Whiten,A.

PY - 2014/11/1

Y1 - 2014/11/1

N2 - Children often "overimitate," comprehensively copying others' actions despite manifest perceptual cues to their causal ineffectuality. The inflexibility of this behavior renders its adaptive significance difficult to apprehend. This study explored the boundaries of overimitation in 3- to 6-year-old children of three distinct cultures: Westernized, urban Australians (N = 64 in Experiment 1; N = 19 in Experiment 2) and remote communities of South African Bushmen (N = 64) and Australian Aborigines (N = 19). Children overimitated at high frequency in all communities and generalized what they had learned about techniques and object affordances from one object to another. Overimitation thus provides a powerful means of acquiring and flexibly deploying cultural knowledge. The potency of such social learning was also documented compared to opportunities for exploration and practice.

AB - Children often "overimitate," comprehensively copying others' actions despite manifest perceptual cues to their causal ineffectuality. The inflexibility of this behavior renders its adaptive significance difficult to apprehend. This study explored the boundaries of overimitation in 3- to 6-year-old children of three distinct cultures: Westernized, urban Australians (N = 64 in Experiment 1; N = 19 in Experiment 2) and remote communities of South African Bushmen (N = 64) and Australian Aborigines (N = 19). Children overimitated at high frequency in all communities and generalized what they had learned about techniques and object affordances from one object to another. Overimitation thus provides a powerful means of acquiring and flexibly deploying cultural knowledge. The potency of such social learning was also documented compared to opportunities for exploration and practice.

U2 - 10.1111/cdev.12265

DO - 10.1111/cdev.12265

M3 - Article

VL - 85

SP - 2169

EP - 2184

JO - Child Development

JF - Child Development

SN - 0009-3920

IS - 6

ER -

Related by author

  1. Payoff- and sex-biased social learning interact in a wild primate population

    Bono, A. E. J., Whiten, A., van Schaik, C., Krützen, M., Eichenberger, F., Schnider, A. & van de Waal, E. 10 Sep 2018 In : Current Biology. 28, 17, p. 2800-2805.e4

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  2. Behavioral conservatism is linked to complexity of behavior in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): implications for cognition and cumulative culture

    Davis, S. J., Schapiro, S. J., Lambeth, S. P., Wood, L. A. & Whiten, A. 19 Jul 2018 In : Journal of Comparative Psychology. Advance Online

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  3. The interaction of social and perceivable causal factors in shaping ‘over-imitation’

    Burdett, E. R. R., McGuigan, N., Harrison, R. & Whiten, A. Jul 2018 In : Cognitive Development. 47, p. 8-18 11 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  4. Chimpanzees demonstrate individual differences in social information use

    Watson, S. K., Vale, G. L., Hopper, L. M., Dean, L. G., Kendal, R. L., Price, E. E., Wood, L. A., Davis, S. J., Schapiro, S. J., Lambeth, S. P. & Whiten, A. 19 Jun 2018 In : Animal Cognition. In press, 12 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

Related by journal

  1. The development of selective copying: children's learning from an expert versus their mother

    Lucas, A. J., Burdett, E. R. R., Burgess, V., Wood, L. A., McGuigan, N., Harris, P. L. & Whiten, A. 7 Nov 2017 In : Child Development. 88, 6, p. 2026-2042 17 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  2. Imitation, collaboration, and their interaction among Western and Indigenous Australian preschool children

    Nielsen, M., Mushin, I., Tomaselli, K. & Whiten, A. 18 May 2016 In : Child Development. 87, 3, p. 795-806 12 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  3. The early emergence of guilt-motivated prosocial behavior

    Vaish, A., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. 7 Nov 2016 In : Child Development. 87, 6, p. 1772-1782 11 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

  4. Learning from their own actions: the unique effect of producing actions on infants’ action understanding

    Gerson, S. & Woodward, A. Feb 2014 In : Child Development. 85, 1, p. 264-277

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

ID: 159773130