Skip to content

Research at St Andrews

Whistle sharing in paired male bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus

Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

Standard

Whistle sharing in paired male bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. / Watwood, S L ; Tyack, P L ; Wells, R S .

In: Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, Vol. 55, No. 6, 04.2004, p. 531-543.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

Harvard

Watwood, SL, Tyack, PL & Wells, RS 2004, 'Whistle sharing in paired male bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus', Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, vol. 55, no. 6, pp. 531-543. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-003-0724-y

APA

Watwood, S. L., Tyack, P. L., & Wells, R. S. (2004). Whistle sharing in paired male bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 55(6), 531-543. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-003-0724-y

Vancouver

Watwood SL, Tyack PL, Wells RS. Whistle sharing in paired male bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2004 Apr;55(6):531-543. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-003-0724-y

Author

Watwood, S L ; Tyack, P L ; Wells, R S . / Whistle sharing in paired male bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. In: Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2004 ; Vol. 55, No. 6. pp. 531-543.

Bibtex - Download

@article{ba603fcf170847e2aa1c35504052b449,
title = "Whistle sharing in paired male bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus",
abstract = "The signature whistle hypothesis states that dolphins produce highly stereotyped, individually distinctive whistles when in isolation. The presence of signature whistles has been called into question by recent studies proposing that dolphins produce a shared, simple upsweep whistle when in isolation, and that whistles produced by socializing dolphins are shared across individuals and social groups. This shared repertoire hypothesis suggests that when two animals produce the same whistle type, it is due to sharing the same common repertoire rather than one animal learning to produce the whistle of another. One difference between studies supporting or denying the existence of signature whistles is the method used to classify whistle types. We examined whistle production by 17 free-ranging bottlenose dolphins while temporarily restrained. We used both a quantitative comparison technique similar to that used to support the shared repertoire hypothesis and human judges to classify whistle types and quantify similarity between types. Contrary to recent studies that emphasize shared whistles, overall whistle sharing between isolated individuals was low (25%) and a simple upsweep did not account for the most common whistle type in half of the animals. Some species of birds, bats, and primates with stable social groups use vocal learning to converge over time to one common group distinctive call type. We examined whistle similarity between adult male dolphins that are partners in a close social alliance in order to test whether vocal learning may enable a similar vocal convergence. Whistle similarity was rated very high between partners and low between non-partners by both the quantitative technique and human observers. This suggests that as in songbirds and some other mammals, adult male bottlenose dolphins may use vocal learning to converge on similar whistles as they develop affiliative social relationships.",
author = "Watwood, {S L} and Tyack, {P L} and Wells, {R S}",
year = "2004",
month = apr,
doi = "10.1007/s00265-003-0724-y",
language = "English",
volume = "55",
pages = "531--543",
journal = "Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology",
issn = "0340-5443",
publisher = "Springer",
number = "6",

}

RIS (suitable for import to EndNote) - Download

TY - JOUR

T1 - Whistle sharing in paired male bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus

AU - Watwood, S L

AU - Tyack, P L

AU - Wells, R S

PY - 2004/4

Y1 - 2004/4

N2 - The signature whistle hypothesis states that dolphins produce highly stereotyped, individually distinctive whistles when in isolation. The presence of signature whistles has been called into question by recent studies proposing that dolphins produce a shared, simple upsweep whistle when in isolation, and that whistles produced by socializing dolphins are shared across individuals and social groups. This shared repertoire hypothesis suggests that when two animals produce the same whistle type, it is due to sharing the same common repertoire rather than one animal learning to produce the whistle of another. One difference between studies supporting or denying the existence of signature whistles is the method used to classify whistle types. We examined whistle production by 17 free-ranging bottlenose dolphins while temporarily restrained. We used both a quantitative comparison technique similar to that used to support the shared repertoire hypothesis and human judges to classify whistle types and quantify similarity between types. Contrary to recent studies that emphasize shared whistles, overall whistle sharing between isolated individuals was low (25%) and a simple upsweep did not account for the most common whistle type in half of the animals. Some species of birds, bats, and primates with stable social groups use vocal learning to converge over time to one common group distinctive call type. We examined whistle similarity between adult male dolphins that are partners in a close social alliance in order to test whether vocal learning may enable a similar vocal convergence. Whistle similarity was rated very high between partners and low between non-partners by both the quantitative technique and human observers. This suggests that as in songbirds and some other mammals, adult male bottlenose dolphins may use vocal learning to converge on similar whistles as they develop affiliative social relationships.

AB - The signature whistle hypothesis states that dolphins produce highly stereotyped, individually distinctive whistles when in isolation. The presence of signature whistles has been called into question by recent studies proposing that dolphins produce a shared, simple upsweep whistle when in isolation, and that whistles produced by socializing dolphins are shared across individuals and social groups. This shared repertoire hypothesis suggests that when two animals produce the same whistle type, it is due to sharing the same common repertoire rather than one animal learning to produce the whistle of another. One difference between studies supporting or denying the existence of signature whistles is the method used to classify whistle types. We examined whistle production by 17 free-ranging bottlenose dolphins while temporarily restrained. We used both a quantitative comparison technique similar to that used to support the shared repertoire hypothesis and human judges to classify whistle types and quantify similarity between types. Contrary to recent studies that emphasize shared whistles, overall whistle sharing between isolated individuals was low (25%) and a simple upsweep did not account for the most common whistle type in half of the animals. Some species of birds, bats, and primates with stable social groups use vocal learning to converge over time to one common group distinctive call type. We examined whistle similarity between adult male dolphins that are partners in a close social alliance in order to test whether vocal learning may enable a similar vocal convergence. Whistle similarity was rated very high between partners and low between non-partners by both the quantitative technique and human observers. This suggests that as in songbirds and some other mammals, adult male bottlenose dolphins may use vocal learning to converge on similar whistles as they develop affiliative social relationships.

U2 - 10.1007/s00265-003-0724-y

DO - 10.1007/s00265-003-0724-y

M3 - Article

VL - 55

SP - 531

EP - 543

JO - Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

JF - Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

SN - 0340-5443

IS - 6

ER -

Related by author

  1. Measuring ambient ocean sound during the COVID-19 pandemic

    Tyack, P. L., Miksis-Olds-Olds, J., Urban, Jr, E. R. & Ausubel, J., 4 Mar 2021, In: Eos, Transactions, American Geophysical Union. 102, 10.1029/2021EO155447.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

  2. Effects of 3 Tesla magnetic resonance imaging exposure on the behavior and orientation of homing pigeons Columba livia domestica

    Párraga, D. G., Tyack, P. L., Marco-Cabedo, V., Crespo-Picazo, J. L., Manteca, X. & Martí-Bonmatí, L., 18 Dec 2020, In: PLoS ONE. 15, 12, 9 p., e0241280.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

  3. On the use of the Lloyd's Mirror effect to infer the depth of vocalizing fin whales

    Pereira, A., Harris, D., Tyack, P. & Matias, L., 1 Nov 2020, In: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 148, 5, p. 3086-3101 16 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

  4. Short-term responses of sperm whales Physeter macrocephalus to the attachment of suction cup tags

    Warren, V. E., Miller, P. J. O. & Tyack, P. L., 9 Jul 2020, In: Marine Ecology Progress Series. 645, p. 219-234 16 p.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

  5. Flash and grab: deep-diving southern elephant seals trigger anti-predator flashes in bioluminescent prey

    Goulet, P., Guinet, C., Campagna, C., Campagna, J., Tyack, P. L. & Johnson, M., 19 May 2020, In: Journal of Experimental Biology. 223, 10, 11 p., jeb.222810.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

Related by journal

  1. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (Journal)

    Monica Arso Civil (Reviewer)

    15 Dec 2016 → …

    Activity: Publication peer-review and editorial work typesPeer review of manuscripts

  2. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (Journal)

    Luke Edward Rendell (Editor)

    2015 → …

    Activity: Publication peer-review and editorial work typesEditor of research journal

  3. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (Journal)

    Kate Arnold (Reviewer)

    2007 → …

    Activity: Publication peer-review and editorial work typesPeer review of manuscripts

  4. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (Journal)

    Vincent Janik (Member of editorial board)

    20052014

    Activity: Publication peer-review and editorial work typesEditor of research journal

  5. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (Journal)

    Anne Magurran (Editor)

    19972002

    Activity: Publication peer-review and editorial work typesEditor of research journal

Related by journal

  1. Individual behavioural traits not social context affects learning about novel objects in archerfish

    Jones, N. A. R., Spence-Jones, H. C., Webster, M. & Rendell, L. E., 22 Feb 2021, In: Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 75, 11 p., 58.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

  2. Effective use of the McNemar test

    Pembury Smith, M. & Ruxton, G. D., 10 Oct 2020, In: Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 74, 9 p., 133.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

  3. Effects of indirect reputation and type of rearing on food choices in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)

    Bueno-Guerra, N., Colell, M. & Call, J., 6 Jun 2020, In: Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 74, 6, 79.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

  4. Grouped circular data in biology: advice for effectively implementing statistical procedures

    Landler, L., Ruxton, G. D. & Malkemper, E. P., 20 Jul 2020, In: Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 74, 8, 8 p., 100.

    Research output: Contribution to journalArticlepeer-review

ID: 20035742

Top